The obvious take home is that one
makes the memory effort, just before having a nap and fall asleep while working
on the material. Then on awakening,
regurgitate the memory, perhaps by writing.
Make a habit of doing this every
night if that is possible. From all that
one should be able to produce a trained memory.
I suspect that this approach
could make memory efforts far more rewarding and one will not be fighting the
drudgery of a lot of it.
Memories take hold better during sleep: study
January 24, 2011 by Marlowe Hood
The best way to not forget a newly learned poem, card trick or algebra
equation may be to take a quick nap, scientists surprised by their own findings
reported.
The best way to not forget a newly learned poem, card trick or algebra
equation may be to take a quick nap, scientists surprised by their own findings
reported Sunday.
In experiments, researchers in Germany showed that the brain is
better during sleep than during wakefulness at
resisting attempts to scramble or corrupt a recent memory.
Their study, published in Nature Neuroscience,
provides new insights into the hugely complex process by which we store and
retrieve deliberately acquired information -- learning, in short.
Earlier research showed that fresh memories, stored temporarily in a
region of the brain called the hippocampus, do not gel
immediately.
It was also known that reactivation of those memories soon after
learning plays a crucial role in their transfer to more permanent storage in
the brain's "hard drive," the neocortex.
During wakefulness, however, this period of reactivation renders the
memories more fragile.
Learning a second poem at this juncture, for example, will likely make
it harder to commit the first one to deep memory.
Bjorn Rasch of the University of Lubeck
in Germany
and three colleagues assumed that the same thing happens when we sleep, and
designed an experiment to find out if they were right.
Twenty-four volunteers were asked to memorise 15 pairs of cards showing
pictures of animals and everyday objects. While performing the exercise, they
were exposed to a slightly unpleasant odour.
Forty minutes later, half the subjects who had stayed awake were asked
to learn a second, slightly different pattern of cards.
Just before starting, they were again made to smell the same odour,
designed to trigger their memory of the first exercise.
The 12 other subjects, meanwhile, did the second exercise after a brief
snooze, during which they were exposed to the odour while in a state called
slow-wave sleep.
Both groups were then tested on the original task.
Much to the surprise of the researchers, the sleep group performed
significantly better, retaining on average 85 percent of the patterns, compared
to 60 percent for those who had remained awake.
"Reactivation of memories had completely different effects on the
state of wakefulness and sleep," said lead author Susanne Diekelmann, also
from the University
of Lubeck .
"Based on brain imaging data, we suggest the reason for this
unexpected result is that already during the first few minutes of sleep, the
transfer from hippocampus to neocortex has been initiated," she said in an
email exchange.
After only 40 minutes of shuteye, significant chunks of memory were
already "downloaded" and stored where they "could no longer be
disrupted by new information that is encoded in the hippocampus," she
explained.
Diekelmann said the positive impact of short periods of sleep on memory
consolidation could have implications for memory-intensive activities such as
language training.
The findings, she said, also point to a strategy for helping victims of
post-traumatic stress syndrome, a debilitating condition caused by extreme
experiences.
The reactivation techniques "might prove useful in re-processing
and un-learning unwanted memories," she said. "And reactivation of
newly learned memories during ensuing sleep could then help
consolidate the desired therapeutic effects for the long-term."
Diekelmann cautioned that computers are an imperfect metaphor for the
way memories are stored in the brain.
"Human memory is absolutely dynamic. Memories are not statically
'archived' in the neocortex but are subject to constant changes by various
influences," she said.
Likewise, the act of remembering does not simply entail
"reading" the stored data, she added. "Recall is a
reconstructive process in which memories can be changed and distorted."
No comments:
Post a Comment