Psychology continues to struggle
to achieve a working paradigm and I notice that the language of spiritualism is
slowly creeping into the research. Thus I
find the word soul used here. It is
probably appropriate as spiritualism is the long traditional empirical tail to
our limited understanding of the minds workings.
Meditation leads to conscious
oversight of the mind’s inputs and allows exercise of some degree of
control. It does not always work but
then what actually does? My own
experiences lead me to consider a program of meditation training as the first
tool that needs to be deployed with any patient suffering from any form of
mental distress. It simply allows the
patient to observe his disease and to develop strategies to counter its baleful
influence.
It does, require a lot of effort
and reinforcement and the establishment of targets in order to achieve
goals. Yet I suspect that most problems
can at least be ameliorated and actively overseen by the patients themselves
once properly trained.
Our Souls Are In Our Eyes, Psychologists Claim
Natalie Wolchover, Life's Little Mysteries Staff Writer
Date: 20 March 2012 Time: 05:28 PM ET
As the cheesy pickup line suggests, your eyes may really be the window
to your soul. According to a new study by Yale University
psychologists, most people intuitively feel as if their "self" —
otherwise known as their soul, or ego — exists in or near their eyes.
In three experiments, the researchers probed preschoolers' and adults'
intuitions about the precise location of the self in the body. The participants
were shown pictures of cartoon characters, and in each picture a small object
(a buzzing fly or snowflake) was positioned near a different section of the
character's body (face or torso or feet, etc.), always at the same distance
away.
The study participants were then asked which pictures showed the object
closest to the body, the hypothesis being that people would interpret the
object as closest when it was near what they intuitively believed to be
the soul's
location.
As reported earlier this month in the journal Cognition, the vast
majority of the 4-year-olds and adults in the study thought the object was
closest to the character when it was near the character's eyes. This was true
even when the cartoon character was a green-skinned alien whose eyes were on
its chest rather than in its head – suggesting that it was the eyes, rather
than the brain, that seemed most closely tied to the soul.
According to lead researcher Christina Starmans of the Mind and
Development Lab at Yale, she and study co-author Paul Bloom designed their
experiment after a conversation in which they discussed intuitively feeling as
if their consciousnesses were "located" near their eyes, and that
objects seemed closest to them when near their eyes. "We set out to test
whether this was a universally shared intuition," Starmans told Life's Little Mysteries.
As it turned out, it was — even among young children. [Take
the test]
"The indirect nature of our method, and the fact that these judgments
are shared by adults and preschoolers, suggests that our results do not reflect
a culturally learned understanding … but might instead be rooted in a more
intuitive or phenomenological sense of where in our bodies we reside," the
authors concluded.
However, experts disagreed about the implications of the research.
Neurologist Robert Burton, author of numerous books and articles on the mind-body connection,
thinks the results don't rule out the possibility that Westerners' sense that
we exist in our eyes is culturally indoctrinated.
Burton, former chief of the division of neurology at University of
California, San Francisco-Mount Zion Hospital, said the most interesting result
of the study seems to have been brushed under the rug by the researchers: It is
that the 4-year-olds and adults didn't actually give the same responses during
the experiment with the alien cartoon character. Almost as many children
thought the buzzing fly was closest to the alien when it was near his eyeless
head than when it was near his eye-bearing chest. Meanwhile, the adults almost
unanimously selected the chest-eyes. "This suggests that something has
transpired during the time between age 4 and adulthood that affects our
understanding of the identity of other people," Burton said.
In other words, it seems we learn to associate identity with
eyes, rather than doing it innately from birth. Perhaps, for example, eyes take
on more importance as we develop awareness of the social cues that other people
convey with their eyes. Or, perhaps it's because adults have learned that it's
good etiquette to make eye-contact.
Furthermore, the study participants may not have interpreted the idea
of the buzzing fly and snowflake being "closer" to a cartoon
characters as meaning that they were closer to its soul or self. Objects look
bigger when they are nearer one's eyes, and this may have confused the
participants into labeling them as "closer." [Gallery:
The Most Amazing Optical Illusions]
Georg Northoff, a neuropsychiatrist at the University of Ottawa, agrees
that the authors' interpretation of their experimental results is
"far-fetched." The issues with this particular study aside, Northoff
said a large body of evidence suggests most people do have a sense of self that
physically manifests itself in their bodies. "We always have the tendency
to locate something and materialize it in the body as mind or as soul," he
wrote in an email. "That seems to be predisposed by the way our brain
works, though the mechanisms remain unclear."
It is also worth noting that the part of the brain in which
self-awareness is thought to arise, called the ventromedial prefrontal cortex,
happens to be located behind the eyes. It is possible, Burton said, that we may "feel" as
if we are physically located near our eyes because our identity emerges in the
neurons there.
Follow Natalie Wolchover on Twitter @nattyover. Follow Life's Little
Mysteries on Twitter @llmysteries,
then join us on Facebook.
No comments:
Post a Comment