It is a bit disconcerting to realize
that one out of a hundred is a psychopath.
The good news is that few actually rise to serious positions in society mostly
because that ultimately entails establishing chains of trust. Those making it past that hurdle are that
much more dangerous.
It is enough to say that I have
met my share but found an effective sieve was to simply postpone a decision for
three months. That was good enough to
flush out the majority without them ever knowing.
We now have evidence of a
biological shortcoming underlying the actual syndrome and perhaps we can expect
a reduction in the damage done by these individuals. The good news is that the majority show themselves
far too fast and can be handled but never trusted.
How to Act Like a Psychopath without Really Trying [Excerpt]
People who don't care—or don't need to care—what others think of them
show how crucial reputation is to civilization. Understanding it could reduce
crime, improve ethical behavior and rein in Wall Street excesses
Editor's note: The following is an excerpt adapted from the book, People
Will Talk: The Surprising Science of Reputation, by John Whitfield (Wiley,
2011). Copyright © John Whitfield
About one in every 100 people doesn’t care what others think of him.
These people are hard to spot. They are usually physically healthy, and their
intelligence is often above average. Yet, in the words of one psychiatrist,
they lie without compunction, cheat, steal, and casually violate any and all
norms of social conduct whenever it suits their whim. They have no concern for
others’ suffering, no remorse when caught, and punishment does little to change
them. They are called psychopaths.
Mental-health professionals have usually treated psychopathic behavior
as a disorder—a large proportion of the prison population, after all, has been
diagnosed with some version of the trait. But viewed from an evolutionary
angle, psychopathy looks more like a feature than a bug. Most people are
cooperative, trusting and generous. This pays off in the long term. It also
creates an opening for those who would rather prey on society than join it.
A psychopath’s deceitful, manipulative, and callous nature equips him
(it’s several times more likely to be a “him”) to fill this niche. Psychopaths’
deficit is in empathy, not reason. They understand morality, but they are
immune to other people’s emotions. There aren't many openings for psychopaths,
because if there were lots of them, there would be no society to plunder.
Evolutionary biologists call this frequency dependence: it means that the rarer
a trait becomes, the more it pays off. This advantage when rare makes the trait
more common, which reduces its advantage. The effect is to keep multiple traits
in balance. Sex is one example of frequency dependence: if males were more
common than females, they would be less likely to find a mate, so it would pay
to have female offspring, pushing the sex ratio back toward equality.
Similarly, mathematical models suggest that if antisocial behavior is rare
enough, it can prosper.
The benefit of psychopathy is that you exploit the altruistic without
the cost of reciprocating. The downside is that you can cheat a person only so
many times. Your victims will also warn their friends about you, and they may
seek revenge. So a psychopath must stay one step ahead of his reputation.
Signs of the psychopath
Psychopaths’ psychology reflects this. They are often drifters. They
have a dread of commitment in work, friendship, or romance. They go for the
quick buck and the one-night stand. They are impulsive and uninhibited, and
bore easily. A psychopath is someone who lives fast and does what he can to
escape the consequences.
A few of us are psychopaths, and a few of us are saints who would never
do a bad thing. Most of us are somewhere in the middle. We are predisposed to
cooperate, but, unconsciously or not, we also weigh up the benefits of cheating
against the likelihood and consequences of getting caught. Concern for our
reputations is one thing that nudges us toward the saintly end of the spectrum.
Yet not every environment triggers the same concern. In a small village
or a hunter-gatherer group, everyone knows everyone else’s business. There’s
slim chance of getting away with anything. It’s easy to resist temptation in
such an environment, and antisocial behavior is relatively rare in such
communities. Urban life, on the other hand, is much more conducive to cheating.
It’s no mystery what conditions make reputation powerful and encourage good
behavior: transparency, accountability, and interdependence. Secrecy, impunity,
and isolation do the reverse, making antisocial behavior more profitable.
The British expense-claims scandal
Two recent scandals illustrate this. In 2009, the expense claims of U.K. Members of
Parliament (MPs) were made public for the first time. These revealed some of
the things the British public had bought for its politicians. The then home
secretary Jacqui Smith had claimed more than £150,000 ($234,000) for furnishing
her second home with everything from two wide-screen TVs to a bath plug and,
most notoriously, two pay-per-view porn movies watched by her husband. The
Conservative MP Douglas Hogg submitted a list of expenses for his country
estate that included £2,200 for cleaning the moat. He also claimed £2.99 for
garbage bags.
Politicians are probably no greedier than other Britons. The problem
was that MPs thought their claims were secret—and they fought to keep them that
way—so short-term self-interest drowned out the voice of reputation. Because
most of their colleagues were doing the same thing—more than half of MPs put in
excessive claims—there seemed to be no risk.
Every elected official, though, relies on connections reaching beyond
the political bubble, in the form of voters. When the expense claims were
revealed these connections became conduits of outrage. Peter Viggers, who
claimed £1,600 ($2,500) for a floating duck house for his garden pond,
described himself as “ashamed and humiliated” (shame being the pain of a
damaged reputation). He was one of more than 120 who decided to stand down at
the 2010 election, the most retirements since World War II. Attempts at reform
have been criticized as piecemeal and grudging, yet the affair's emotional
impact is undeniable.
No apologies from investment bankers
In contrast, look at how investment bankers have responded to the
stratospherically expensive rescue of their industry. The drop in their
reputations was every bit as precipitous as it was for British MPs. And yet
judging from the bonuses they continue to reward themselves and their efforts
to resist regulation, they are unabashed. The journalist Andrew Ross Sorkin has
said that when he asked leading figures in the industry whether they felt any
remorse, “The answer, almost unequivocally, was no.” Bankers, he says, see
themselves more as “survivors” of the crisis than its cause.
British MPs could say the same. Yet while politicians have to worry
about their reputations with the public, bankers have few ties to non-bankers,
working long hours and socializing mainly with their colleagues. They don't
need to worry about those on the outside, and within banking, the behavior that
led to the meltdown was somewhere between normal and virtuous, so no need to
feel bad. If some bankers look like psychopaths, that’s not because they're
bad, it's because they don’t need to care what we think.
The chance to make a lot of money quickly is another reason to behave
selfishly. The economist Will Hutton blamed the banking crisis on huge bonuses
that “trashed the need for individuals to worry about integrity.” Such people,
he wrote, “don’t need to be concerned about their reputations; they just need
one deal or one year at the top and they need never work again.”
But you can overestimate the power of this temptation. British MPs sold
their reputations for a piffling return—one MP submitted a claim for a hot
drink he had bought in the House of Commons tearoom. Psychological experiments
suggest that pretty much everyone, psychopaths aside, cares about what others
think of them. Even people who before the experiment say they don't care about
public opinion suffer a knock to their self-esteem if they are snubbed in the
name of science. And, perhaps most important, status is relative: if everyone
you know makes $20 million, walking away with your $5 million will feel like
failure.
The problem with too much money
Extreme wealth damages society not because it corrupts, but because it
isolates. Inequality severs connections, splitting people into groups whose
members cannot influence one another. This may be one reason why more unequal
societies suffer more crime and mental illness, regardless of their wealth.
Making connections, on the other hand, forces people to confront the
consequences of their actions. Restorative justice, in which criminals meet
their victims, has a good record of preventing re-offending, and a project
called Operation Ceasefire, pioneered in Boston and applied in other cities in
the U.S. and U.K., has reduced gun- and gang-crime partly by bringing victims
and perpetrators together. Our social natures make it harder for us to hurt
people we see as like us.
The problem in the financial sector was not that its members sold their
reputations. It was that reputations were not at risk. The incentives to gamble
so dwarfed the penalties for failure that it was difficult for anyone,
regardless of how dishonest or incompetent he might be, to damage anyone else.
This gave bankers little reason to worry about their professional reputations,
and little reason to regulate themselves. In contrast, the British expenses
scandal caused a backlash against all mainstream politicians. The guilty
dragged down the innocent, giving everyone a reason to reform.
In a prescient article written in 2005, the London banker Stanislas Yassukovich said that
investment banks had come to see scandals as an advertisement, rather than an
indictment. “Reputational risk is no longer a meaningful element of corporate
policy,” he wrote. “It is no longer a question of knowing right from wrong; it
is a matter of knowing what you can get away with and what might be the cost of
getting caught.” When the answers to these questions are, respectively,
“plenty” and “not much,” it’s hard for anyone to behave well.
When we don’t cheat, it is often because the environment makes cheating
a bad move. The challenge for governments is to align the financial sector's
self-interest with the public good]. If bankers were better connected to the
rest of us, and if they harmed themselves — or better still, each other — when
their decisions harmed society, they would police one another more keenly.
Several banks have begun paying bonuses in shares that can be cashed in only
after a number of years, to give employees a stake in the long-term health of
their institutions. Perhaps even more effective would be to give them shares in
one another’s companies, so that a fund manager was tending the bonus of the
guy up the street. That might make money’s mad scientists think twice before
blowing up the lab.
ABOUT THE AUTHOR(S)
John Whitfield is a London-based science writer.
1 comment:
"few actually rise to serious positions in society"
Really, arclein? Are you truly that naive?
This world is being run by psychopaths, and those too blind to see that are part of the problem.
Watch: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=MgGyvxqYSbE
Post a Comment