It becomes clear
to me that it is plausible to train wild elephants to work human herders who
look after their communication needs and guide their material life. It will require teaching the young over two
to three years and from that the whole herd in time to work with human herders.
Forest
management is a natural niche in which hard working elephants can clear large
tracts to everyone’s mutual benefit.
This is not
going to be easy and it will not be a case of riding African elephants in the
same manner as tamed Indian elephants.
Yet through pointing and shared tasks, we have a clear opening to
exploit and possibly end the ongoing threat to their lives.
Elephants Get
the Point
10.10.13
The next time you need to show an elephant where
something is, just point. Chances are he’ll understand what you mean.
New research shows elephants spontaneously understand the
communicative intent of human pointing and can use it as a cue to find food.
Richard Byrne and Anna Smet of the University of St.
Andrews tested 11 African elephants on what’s known as the object-choice task.
In this task, a food reward is hidden in one of several containers and the
experimenter signals which one by pointing to it.
People understand pointing, even as young children.
But the track record of other animals on the object-choice task is mixed.
Domesticated animals, such as dogs, cats, and horses, tend to perform better
than wild ones. Even our closest relative, the chimpanzee, typically
struggles to understand pointing when it’s used by human caretakers.
What’s so
remarkable about the elephants’ success on the object-choice task is that they
did it spontaneously. Byrne says
that in studies of other species, the animals have had the opportunity to
learn the task. This is usually during the experiment itself, which consists of
a prolonged series of tests over which the animals come to realize they will
get rewarded with food if they follow the line of the human’s pointing.
But the elephants performed as well on the first
trial as on later tests and showed no signs of learning over the course of the
experiments. The elephants Byrne and Smet tested are used to take tourists on
elephant-back rides in southern Africa. They were trained to follow vocal
commands only, never gestures. Smet recorded the behavior of the elephants’
handlers over several months and found they never pointed their arms for the
elephants. What’s more, the elephants’ ability to understand human pointing did
not vary with how long they had lived with people, nor with whether they were
captive-born or wild-born. “If they have learned to follow pointing from
their past experiences, it’s mystery when and how,” Byrne says. “Rather, it
seems they do it naturally.”
In the experiment, Byrne and Smet varied several
parameters that often affect children’s and animals’ performance on the task:
whether the pointing arm was nearest the correct choice or not; whether the
pointer’s arm crossed the body or was always on the side of what was pointed
at; and whether the arm broke the silhouette from the elephant’s viewpoint or
not. None of these made any difference. Even when the experimenter stood closer
to the wrong location than the correct location, the elephants performed a
little worse but still mostly responded to where her arm was pointing.
The only condition that truly stymied the elephants
was when the experimenter simply looked at the correct location without
pointing. Byrne says that elephant eyesight is poor compared to our own, and
researchers who work with elephants have commented on how bad they are at
identifying things by sight. “It would perhaps have been surprising if they
had spontaneously responded to the rather subtle movements of a small primate’s
head!” Byrne says.
Elephants are only distantly related to humans,
which means that the ability to understand pointing likely evolved separately
in both species, and not in a shared ancestor. But why would elephants attend
to and understand pointing? One thing elephants do share with humans is that
they live in a complex and extensive social network in which cooperation and
communication with others play a critical role. Byrne and Smet speculate that
pointing relates to something elephants do naturally in their society. “The
most likely possibility is that they regularly interpret trunk gestures as
pointing to places in space,” Byrne says. Elephants do make many prominent
trunk gestures, and Byrne and Smet are currently trying to determine if those motions
act as “points” in elephant society.
Reference:
Smet, Anna F. and Byrne, Richard W. (2013). African Elephants Can Use Human Pointing Cues to Find Hidden Food. Current Biology http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.037
Smet, Anna F. and Byrne, Richard W. (2013). African Elephants Can Use Human Pointing Cues to Find Hidden Food. Current Biology http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.037
No comments:
Post a Comment