I have posted on this before and
it has been pretty clear that cats were indicated as an agency for odd mental
behavior and even schizophrenia in cases.
Enough for this trained scientists to pick up the trail and provide us
with a clear observable and confirmable path of causation.
We are at the same point that we
were when the source of stomach ulcers were first discovered.
All we need now is a clear
protocol that eliminates the disease vector, although I suspect simple
avoidance of cats for a specified time will allow the strain to die out as the parasite
cannot reproduce in our bodies.
I have observed a wide range of
low grade mental issues with folks that are and were best explained as a low
grade infection never understood as such.
In some, low grade physical issues also manifested themselves. Thus the medical fraternity needs to actually
consider the presence of a parasite as a first causation and the most likely also.
How Your Cat Is Making You Crazy
Jaroslav Flegr is no kook. And yet, for years, he suspected his mind
had been taken over by parasites that had invaded his brain. So the prolific
biologist took his science-fiction hunch into the lab. What he’s now discovering
will startle you. Could tiny organisms carried by house cats be creeping into
our brains, causing everything from car wrecks to schizophrenia? A biologist’s
science- fiction hunch is gaining credence and shaping the emerging science of
mind- controlling parasites.
NO ONE WOULD accuse Jaroslav Flegr of being a conformist. A
self-described “sloppy dresser,” the 63-year-old Czech scientist has the
contemplative air of someone habitually lost in thought, and his
still-youthful, square-jawed face is framed by frizzy red hair that encircles
his head like a ring of fire.
Certainly Flegr’s thinking is jarringly unconventional. Starting in the
early 1990s, he began to suspect that a single-celled parasite in the protozoan
family was subtly manipulating his personality, causing him to behave in
strange, often self-destructive ways. And if it was messing with his mind, he
reasoned, it was probably doing the same to others.
The parasite, which is excreted by cats in their feces, is called Toxoplasma
gondii (T. gondii or Toxo for short) and is the microbe
that causes toxoplasmosis—the reason pregnant women are told to avoid cats’
litter boxes. Since the 1920s, doctors have recognized that a woman who becomes
infected during pregnancy can transmit the disease to the fetus, in some cases
resulting in severe brain damage or death. T. gondii is also a major
threat to people with weakened immunity: in the early days of the AIDS epidemic,
before good antiretroviral drugs were developed, it was to blame for the
dementia that afflicted many patients at the disease’s end stage. Healthy
children and adults, however, usually experience nothing worse than brief
flu-like symptoms before quickly fighting off the protozoan, which thereafter
lies dormant inside brain cells—or at least that’s the standard medical wisdom.
But if Flegr is right, the “latent” parasite may be quietly tweaking
the connections between our neurons, changing our response to frightening
situations, our trust in others, how outgoing we are, and even our preference
for certain scents. And that’s not all. He also believes that the organism
contributes to car crashes, suicides, and mental disorders such as
schizophrenia. When you add up all the different ways it can harm us, says
Flegr, “Toxoplasma might even kill as many people as malaria, or at least a
million people a year.”
An evolutionary biologist at Charles
University in Prague , Flegr has pursued this theory for
decades in relative obscurity. Because he struggles with English and is not
much of a conversationalist even in his native tongue, he rarely travels to
scientific conferences. That “may be one of the reasons my theory is not better
known,” he says. And, he believes, his views may invite deep-seated opposition.
“There is strong psychological resistance to the possibility that human
behavior can be influenced by some stupid parasite,” he says. “Nobody likes to
feel like a puppet. Reviewers [of my scientific papers] may have been
offended.” Another more obvious reason for resistance, of course, is that
Flegr’s notions sound an awful lot like fringe science, right up there with UFO
sightings and claims of dolphins telepathically communicating with humans.
But after years of being ignored or discounted, Flegr is starting to
gain respectability. Psychedelic as his claims may sound, many researchers,
including such big names in neuroscience as Stanford’s Robert Sapolsky, think
he could well be onto something. Flegr’s “studies are well conducted, and I can
see no reason to doubt them,” Sapolsky tells me. Indeed, recent findings from
Sapolsky’s lab and British groups suggest that the parasite is capable of extraordinary
shenanigans. T. gondii, reports Sapolsky, can turn a rat’s strong innate
aversion to cats into an attraction, luring it into the jaws of its No. 1
predator. Even more amazing is how it does this: the organism rewires circuits
in parts of the brain that deal with such primal emotions as fear, anxiety, and
sexual arousal. “Overall,” says Sapolsky, “this is wild, bizarre neurobiology.”
Another academic heavyweight who takes Flegr seriously is the schizophrenia
expert E. Fuller Torrey, director of the Stanley Medical Research Institute, in
Maryland . “I
admire Jaroslav for doing [this research],” he says. “It’s obviously not
politically correct, in the sense that not many labs are doing it. He’s done it
mostly on his own, with very little support. I think it bears looking at. I
find it completely credible.”
What’s more, many experts think T. gondii may be far from the
only microscopic puppeteer capable of pulling our strings. “My guess is that
there are scads more examples of this going on in mammals, with parasites we’ve
never even heard of,” says Sapolsky.
Familiar to most of us, of course, is the rabies virus. On the verge of
killing a dog, bat, or other warm-blooded host, it stirs the animal into a rage
while simultaneously migrating from the nervous system to the creature’s
saliva, ensuring that when the host bites, the virus will live on in a new
carrier. But aside from rabies, stories of parasites commandeering the behavior
of large-brained mammals are rare. The far more common victims of parasitic
mind control—at least the ones we know about—are fish, crustaceans, and legions
of insects, according to Janice Moore, a behavioral biologist at Colorado State University .
“Flies, ants, caterpillars, wasps, you name it—there are truckloads of them
behaving weirdly as a result of parasites,” she says.
Consider Polysphincta gutfreundi, a parasitic wasp that grabs hold
of an orb spider and attaches a tiny egg to its belly. A wormlike larva emerges
from the egg, and then releases chemicals that prompt the spider to abandon
weaving its familiar spiral web and instead spin its silk thread into a special
pattern that will hold the cocoon in which the larva matures. The “possessed”
spider even crochets a specific geometric design in the net, camouflaging the
cocoon from the wasp’s predators.
Flegr himself traces his life’s work to another master of mind control.
Almost 30 years ago, as he was reading a book by the British evolutionary
biologist Richard Dawkins, Flegr was captivated by a passage describing how a
flatworm turns an ant into its slave by invading the ant’s nervous system. A
drop in temperature normally causes ants to head underground, but the infected
insect instead climbs to the top of a blade of grass and clamps down on it,
becoming easy prey for a grazing sheep. “Its mandibles actually become locked
in that position, so there’s nothing the ant can do except hang there in the
air,” says Flegr. The sheep grazes on the grass and eats the ant; the worm
gains entrance into the ungulate’s gut, which is exactly where it needs to be
in order to complete—as theLion King song goes—the circle of life. “It was
the first I learned about this kind of manipulation, so it made a big
impression on me,” Flegr says.
After he read the book, Flegr began to make a connection that, he
readily admits, others might find crazy: his behavior, he noticed, shared
similarities with that of the reckless ant. For example, he says, he thought
nothing of crossing the street in the middle of dense traffic, “and if cars
honked at me, I didn’t jump out of the way.” He also made no effort to hide his
scorn for the Communists who ruled Czechoslovakia for most of his
early adulthood. “It was very risky to openly speak your mind at that time,” he
says. “I was lucky I wasn’t imprisoned.” And during a research stint in eastern
Turkey ,
when the strife-torn region frequently erupted in gunfire, he recalls being
“very calm.” In contrast, he says, “my colleagues were terrified. I wondered
what was wrong with myself.”
His bewilderment continued until 1990, when he joined the biology
faculty of Charles
University . As it
happened, the 650-year-old institution had long been a world leader in
documenting the health effects of T. gondii, as well as developing methods
for detecting the parasite. In fact, just as Flegr was arriving, his colleagues
were searching for infected individuals on whom to test their improved
diagnostic kits, which is how he came to be asked one day to roll up his sleeve
and donate blood. He discovered that he had the parasite—and just possibly, he
thought, the key to his baffling self-destructive streak.
He delved into T. gondii’s life cycle. After an infected cat
defecates, Flegr learned, the parasite is typically picked up from the soil by
scavenging or grazing animals—notably rodents, pigs, and cattle—all of which
then harbor it in their brain and other body tissues. Humans, on the other
hand, are exposed not only by coming into contact with litter boxes, but also,
he found, by drinking water contaminated with cat feces, eating unwashed
vegetables, or, especially in Europe, by consuming raw or undercooked meat.
Hence the French, according to Flegr, with their love of steak prepared saignant—literally,
“bleeding”—can have infection rates as high as 55 percent. (Americans will be
happy to hear that the parasite resides in far fewer of them, though a still
substantial portion: 10 to 20 percent.) Once inside an animal or human host,
the parasite then needs to get back into the cat, the only place where it can
sexually reproduce—and this is when, Flegr believed, behavioral manipulation
might come into play.
Researchers had already observed
a few peculiarities about rodents with T. gondii that bolstered
Flegr’s theory. The infected rodents were much more active in running wheels
than uninfected rodents were, suggesting that they would be more-attractive
targets for cats, which are drawn to fast-moving objects. They also were less
wary of predators in exposed spaces. Little, however, was known about how the
latent infection might influence humans, because we and other large mammals
were widely presumed to be accidental hosts, or, as scientists are fond of
putting it, a “dead end” for the parasite. But even if we were never part of
the parasite’s life cycle, Flegr reasoned, mammals from mouse to man share the
vast majority of their genes, so we might, in a case of mistaken identity,
still be vulnerable to manipulations by the parasite.
The parasite T. gondii, seen here, may be changing connections between
our neurones, altering how we act and feel. (Dennis Kunkel Microscropy,
Inc./Visuals Unlimited/Corbis Images)
In the Soviet-stunted economy, animal studies were way beyond Flegr’s
research budget. But fortunately for him, 30 to 40 percent of Czechs had the
latent form of the disease, so plenty of students were available “to serve as
very cheap experimental animals.” He began by giving them and their
parasite-free peers standardized personality tests—an inexpensive, if somewhat
crude, method of measuring differences between the groups. In addition, he used
a computer-based test to assess the reaction times of participants, who were
instructed to press a button as soon as a white square popped up anywhere
against the dark background of the monitor.
The subjects who tested positive for the parasite had significantly
delayed reaction times. Flegr was especially surprised to learn, though, that
the protozoan appeared to cause many sex-specific changes in personality.
Compared with uninfected men, males who had the parasite were more introverted,
suspicious, oblivious to other people’s opinions of them, and inclined to
disregard rules. Infected women, on the other hand, presented in exactly the
opposite way: they were more outgoing, trusting, image-conscious, and
rule-abiding than uninfected women.
The findings were so bizarre that Flegr initially assumed his data must
be flawed. So he tested other groups—civilian and military populations. Again,
the same results. Then, in search of more corroborating evidence, he brought
subjects in for further observation and a battery of tests, in which they were
rated by someone ignorant of their infection status. To assess whether
participants valued the opinions of others, the rater judged how well dressed
they appeared to be. As a measure of gregariousness, participants were asked
about the number of friends they’d interacted with over the past two weeks. To
test whether they were prone to being suspicious, they were asked, among other
things, to drink an unidentified liquid.
The results meshed well with the questionnaire findings. Compared with
uninfected people of the same sex, infected men were more likely to wear
rumpled old clothes; infected women tended to be more meticulously attired,
many showing up for the study in expensive, designer-brand clothing. Infected
men tended to have fewer friends, while infected women tended to have more. And
when it came to downing the mystery fluid, reports Flegr, “the infected males
were much more hesitant than uninfected men. They wanted to know why they had
to do it. Would it harm them?” In contrast, the infected women were the most
trusting of all subjects. “They just did what they were told,” he says.
Why men and women reacted so differently to the parasite still
mystified him. After consulting the psychological literature, he started to
suspect that heightened anxiety might be the common denominator underlying
their responses. When under emotional strain, he read, women seek solace
through social bonding and nurturing. In the lingo of psychologists, they’re
inclined to “tend and befriend.” Anxious men, on the other hand, typically
respond by withdrawing and becoming hostile or antisocial. Perhaps he was looking
at flip sides of the same coin.
Closer inspection of Flegr’s reaction-time results revealed that
infected subjects became less attentive and slowed down a minute or so into the
test. This suggested to him that Toxoplasma might have an adverse
impact on driving, where constant vigilance and fast reflexes are critical. He
launched two major epidemiological studies in the Czech Republic ,
one of men and women in the general population and another of mostly male
drivers in the military. Those who tested positive for the parasite, both
studies showed, were about two and a half times as likely to be in a traffic
accident as their uninfected peers.
WHEN I MET Flegr for the first time, last September, at his office
on the third floor of Charles
University ’s Biological
Sciences building, I was expecting something of a wild man. But once you get
past the riotous red hair, his style is understated. Thin and slight of build,
he’s soft-spoken, precise with his facts, and—true to his Toxostatus—clad
in old sneakers, faded bell-bottom jeans, and a loose-fitting button-up shirt.
As our conversation proceeds, I discover that his latest findings have
become—to quote Alice
in Wonderland—“curiouser and curiouser,” which may explain why his forehead has
the deep ruts of a chronic worrier, or someone perpetually perplexed.
He’s published some data, he tells me, that suggest infected males
might have elevated testosterone levels. Possibly for that reason, women shown
photos of these men rate them as more masculine than pictures of uninfected
men. “I want to investigate this more closely to see if it’s true,” he says.
“Also, it could be women find infected men more attractive. That’s something
else we hope to test.”
Meanwhile, two Turkish studies have replicated his studies linking Toxoplasma to
traffic accidents. With up to one-third of the world infected with the
parasite, Flegr now calculates that T. gondii is a likely factor in
several hundred thousand road deaths each year. In addition, reanalysis of his
personality-questionnaire data revealed that, just like him, many other people
who have the latent infection feel intrepid in dangerous situations. “Maybe,”
he says, “that’s another reason they get into traffic accidents. They don’t
have a normal fear response.”
It’s almost impossible to hear about Flegr’s research without wondering
whether you’re infected—especially if, like me, you’re a cat owner, favor very
rare meat, and identify even a little bit with your Toxo sex
stereotype. So before coming to Prague ,
I’d gotten tested for the parasite, but I didn’t yet know the results. It
seemed a good time to see what his intuition would tell me. “Can you guess from
observing someone whether they have the parasite—myself, for example?,” I ask.
“No,” he says, “the parasite’s effects on personality are very subtle.”
If, as a woman, you were introverted before being infected, he says, the
parasite won’t turn you into a raving extrovert. It might just make you a little
less introverted. “I’m very typical of Toxoplasma males,” he
continues. “But I don’t know whether my personality traits have anything to do
with the infection. It’s impossible to say for any one individual. You usually
need about 50 people who are infected and 50 who are not, in order to see a
statistically significant difference. The vast majority of people will have no
idea they’re infected.”
Still, he concedes, the parasite could be very bad news for a small
percentage of people—and not just those who might be at greater risk for car
accidents. Many schizophrenia patients show shrinkage in parts of their
cerebral cortex, and Flegr thinks the protozoan may be to blame for that. He
hands me a recently published paper on the topic that he co-authored with
colleagues at Charles
University , including a
psychiatrist named Jiri Horacek. Twelve of 44 schizophrenia patients who
underwent MRI scans, the team found, had reduced gray matter in the brain—and
the decrease occurred almost exclusively in those who tested positive for T.
gondii. After reading the abstract, I must look stunned, because Flegr smiles
and says, “Jiri had the same response. I don’t think he believed it could be
true.” When I later speak with Horacek, he admits to having been skeptical
about Flegr’s theory at the outset. When they merged the MRI results with the
infection data, however, he went from being a doubter to being a believer. “I
was amazed at how pronounced the effect was,” he says. “To me that suggests the
parasite may trigger schizophrenia in genetically susceptible people.”
Great stuff; wow
ReplyDeleteThis is a great article. Wondering if there is a way to pass on the info that there are people with schizophrenia who use high dose niacin to great effect, we are talking 3 gm per day. Could it protect from Toxo?
ReplyDelete